Healthcare CEO Killer Inscribed These Words on the Bullet Casings
Trump Needs to Pardon All the J6 Political Prisoners on Day One
Here's How Many GOP Senators Are Now Opposed to Pete Hegseth's Defense Secretary...
Why Esquire Magazine Was Forced to Retract a Column About Hunter Biden's Pardon
Fire Your Whiny Employees Too Upset Over The Election To Do Their Work
The Hunter Biden Pardon
Biden Breaks His Promise and Pardons His Son -- Yeah, But Trump!
The Milei Revolution
Trump Calls Out The Wall Street Journal As He Redefines The Media Landscape
End the IRS's Worldwide Tax Grab
Kash Patel for FBI? Yes.
There Is One Man the Trump Administration Needs to Help Tame the Bureaucratic...
Trump Will Make Energy Affordable Again
Robert F. Kennedy Jr.: Will He Make America Healthy Again?
Supreme Court Hears Historic Argument on Transgender Rights
OPINION

New Study Of Wrongful Convictions Shows The Problem Goes Beyond Misconduct By Police And Prosecutors

The opinions expressed by columnists are their own and do not necessarily represent the views of Townhall.com.
Advertisement
Advertisement
Advertisement
LightFieldStudios/iStock/Getty Images Plus

Last week, an Oklahoma judge freed Corey Atchison, who had spent 28 years in prison for a murder he has always said he did not commit, after concluding that he had been convicted based on the false testimony of "purported eyewitnesses" who had been "coerced" by prosecutors. The next day, an Idaho judge exonerated Christopher Tapp, who had served more than two decades for rape and murder, after DNA evidence implicated another man, who confessed to the crimes.

Advertisement

While cases like these often feature wrongdoing by individual prosecutors and police officers, a new study suggests the problem is deeper. After analyzing 50 wrongful convictions and other investigative failures, Texas State University criminologists Kim Rossmo and Joycelyn Pollock found that confirmation bias, reinforced by groupthink and strong incentives to quickly identify the perpetrators of highly publicized crimes, figures prominently in the mistakes that send innocent people to prison.

Once police decide they have the right suspect, Rossmo and Pollock report in the Northeastern University Law Review, they tend to develop "tunnel vision" that obscures other possibilities. They become focused on building a case against the person they've decided is guilty, ignore or minimize countervailing evidence and interpret ambiguous evidence in a way that supports their initial conclusions.

After Angela Correa, a 15-year-old high school student in Peekskill, New York, was raped and strangled to death in 1989, for example, police quickly settled on one of her classmates, Jeffrey Deskovic, as their sole suspect. "Suffering from tunnel vision, detectives pursued a single-minded course of action designed to get Deskovic to confess," Rossmo and Pollock write. "Police did not look for other suspects despite the presence of exculpatory physical evidence. In a classic confirmation bias pattern, detectives changed their theory of the case when the DNA test results came back excluding Deskovic."

Advertisement

Deskovic was convicted in 1990 based on a false confession he later retracted. For years, the Westchester County district attorney, Jeanine Pirro, now a Fox News host who opines on justice, rejected Deskovic's requests to compare the DNA evidence against a criminal database. Deskovic was not exonerated until 2006, after he had served 16 years in prison, when a new DA approved testing that identified the actual perpetrator.

Christopher Tapp's conviction also was based on a false confession that was contradicted by DNA evidence, the pattern in about a third of the 367 cases in which the Innocence Project has used such evidence to clear people who were wrongly convicted. "Interrogations are not a quest for information," observes the Innocence Project's Vanessa Potkin. "The purpose is to get an admission."

In another case that Rossmo and Pollock examined, Bruce Lisker was wrongly convicted in 1985 of stabbing his mother to death at their home in Sherman Oaks, California. He was not released until 2009, at which point he had served 26 years, after a judge determined that he had been convicted based on false evidence, including the testimony of a jailhouse snitch police knew was unreliable.

"Investigators coerced a confession (quickly recanted) from the 17-year-old teenager through the offer of a plea bargain," Rossmo and Pollock write. "A rush to judgment followed by tunnel vision led to confirmation bias. Exculpatory evidence was ignored, while the alibi of an alternative and viable suspect was never checked despite inconsistencies in his story."

Advertisement

Confirmation bias is common and hard to root out. Rossmo and Pollock recommend training in "cognitive de-biasing," better evidence procedures, closer supervision and a general awareness of cognitive biases and the factors that let them run riot.

Rossmo and Pollock also note that the "probable cause" standard for arresting someone, which creates momentum toward a conviction, is a low bar that does not require showing the suspect is more likely than not to be guilty. "A probable cause that is not probable is inconsistent with both language and mathematics," they write. "The most certain way to prevent a wrongful conviction is to minimize wrongful arrests of innocent people."

Jacob Sullum is a senior editor at Reason magazine. Follow him on Twitter: @JacobSullum.

Join the conversation as a VIP Member

Recommended

Trending on Townhall Videos